Lorem ipsum dolor sit amet, consectetur adipiscing elit. Ut elit tellus, luctus nec ullamcorper mattis, pulvinar dapibus leo.

RESOURCES

THE SCIENTIFIC EVIDENCE
Direct Evidence for the SMART Approach

May, R. J., Tyndall, I., McTiernan, A., Roderique-Davies, G., & McLoughlin, S. (2022). The impact of the SMART program on cognitive and academic skills: A systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of Educational Technology, 53, 1244– 1261.

 

McLoughlin S, Tyndall I, Pereira A, Mulhern T. 2020. Non-verbal IQ Gains from Relational Operant Training Explain Variance in Educational Attainment: An Active-Controlled Feasibility Study. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement.

 

McLoughlin S, Tyndall I, Pereira A. 2020. Relational Operant Skills Training Increases Standardized Matrices Scores in Adolescents: A Stratified Active-Controlled Trial. Journal of Behavioral Education.

 

Amd, M., & Roche, B. (2018). Assessing the effects of a relational training intervention on fluid intelligence among a sample of socially disadvantaged children in Bangladesh. The Psychological Record, 68(2), 141–149.

 

Cassidy, S., Roche, B., Colbert, D., Stewart, I., & Grey, I. (2016). A relational frame skills training intervention to increase general intelligence and scholastic aptitude. Learning and Individual Differences, 47, 222–235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.03.001.

 

Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & Hayes, S. C. (2011). A relational frame training intervention to raise Intelligence Quotients: A pilot study. The Psychological Record, 61, 173–198. https://doi.org/10.1007/ BF03395755.

 

Colbert, D., Tyndall, I., Roche, B., & Cassidy, S. (2018). Can SMART training really increase Intelligence? A Replication Study. Journal of Behavioral Education, 27, 509-531. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-018-9302-2

 

Hayes, J., & Stewart, I. (2016). Comparing the effects of derived relational training and computer coding on intellectual potential in school-age children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 397–411. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12114.

 

McLoughlin, S., Tyndall, I., & Pereira, A. (2018). A brief relational operant training program: Analyses of response latencies and intelligence. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 19 (2). 228-246. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2018.1507087.

 

Thirus, J., Starbrink, M., & Jansson, B. (2016). Relational frame theory, mathematical and logical skills: A multiple exemplar training intervention to enhance intellectual performance. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 16(2), 141–155.

 

Presti, G., Torregrosssa, S., Migliore, D., Roche, B., Cumbo, E. (2018). Relational Training Intervention as add-on therapy to current specific treatments in patients with mild-to-moderate Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Psychology and Neuroscience, 3, 88-97.

 

Vizcaíno-Torres, R.M., Ruiz, F., Luciano, C., López-López, J., Barbero-Rubio, A., & Gil, E. (2015). The effect of relational training on intelligence quotient. A case study. Psicothema, 27(2), 120- 127. https://doi.org/ 10.7334/psicothema2014.149.

Theoretical Background to SMART

McLoughlin, S., Tyndall, I., & Pereira, A. (2020), Convergence of multiple fields on a relational reasoning approach to cognition, Intelligence, 83: 101491.

Andrews, G., & Halford, G. S. (1998). Children’s ability to make transitive inferences: The importance of premise integration and structural complexity. Cognitive Development, 13, 479–513. https://doi. org/10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90004-1.

 

Barnes-Holmes, D., Finn, M., McEnteggart, C., Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2017). Derived Stimulus Relations and Their Role in a Behavior-Analytic Account of Human Language and Cognition. Perspectives on Behavior Science, 41, 155–173. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-017-0124-7

 

Cassidy, S., Roche, B. & O’Hora, D. (2010). Relational Frame Theory and human intelligence. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 11, 37-51.

 

Halford, G., Wilson, W., & Phillips, S. (2010). Relational knowledge: The foundation of higher cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(11), 497–505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.08.005.

 

Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (Eds.) (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Kluwer Academic.

 

Hayes, J., Stewart, I 7 McElwee, J. (2016). Assessing and Training Young Children in Same and Different Relations Using the Relational Evaluation Procedure (REP). The Psychological Record, 66, 547–561. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-016-0191-2

 

Marr, M. (2015). Mathematics as verbal behavior. Behavioural Processes, 113, 75–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.01.005.

 

Rehfeldt, R. A & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2009). Derived Relational Responding Applications for Learners with Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities: A Progressive Guide to Change. San Francisco: New Harbinger.

 

Roche, B., Cassidy, S. & Stewart, I. (2013). Nurturing genius: Realizing a foundational aim of Psychology. In Kashdan, T & Ciarrochi, J. (Eds.), Cultivating well-being: Treatment innovations in Positive Psychology, Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, and beyond, pp. 267-302. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.

 

Stewart, I., McLoughlin, S., Mulhern, T., Ming, S., & Kirsten, A. (in press). Assessing and teaching complex relational operants: Analogy and hierarchy. In R. Rehfeldt, J. Tarbox, M. Fryling & L. Hayes (Eds.), Applied Behavior Analysis of Language and Cognition. New Harbinger.
Stewart, I., Tarbox, J., Roche, B., & O’Hora, D. (2013). Education, intellectual development, and relational frame theory. In S. Dymond & B. Roche (Eds.), Advances in relational frame theory: Research & application (pp. 178–198). Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.

Kirsten, E.B. & Stewart, I. (2022). Analogical relations. In T. Mulhern, Relational Frame Theory Made Simple. Springer.

 

Kirsten, E. B., Stewart, I., & McElwee, J. (2022). Testing and Training Analogical Relational Responding in Children with and Without Autism. The Psychological Record. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-021-00493-8

 

Kirsten, E. B., Stewart, I., & McElwee, J. (2021). Testing and Training Analogical Responding in Young Children Using Relational Frame Theory. The Psychological Record. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-021-00468-9

 

Kirsten, E. B., & Stewart, I. (2021). Assessing the development of relational framing in young children. The Psychological Record. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-021-00457-y

 

Stewart, I., McLoughlin, S., Mulhern, T., Ming, S., & Kirsten, E. B. (2020). Assessing and teaching complex relational operants: Analogy and hierarchy. In R. Rehfeldt, J. Tarbox, M. Fryling, & L. Hayes (Eds.), Applied Behavior Analysis of Language and Cognition. New Harbinger.

 

Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & O’Hora, D. (2010). Relational Frame Theory and Human Intelligence. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 11(1), 37–51. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2 010.11434333

 

Colbert, D., Barrett, S. Malone, A & Roche, B. (In press, 2019). The Relational Abilities Index+: Initial Validation of a Functionally Understood Proxy Measure for Intelligence. Perspectives on Behavioral Science. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-019-00197-z

 

Colbert, D., Dobutowitsch, M., Roche, B., & Brophy, C. (2017). The proxy-measurement of intelligence quotients using a relational skills abilities index. Learning and Individual Differences. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.010.

 

Dixon, M., Whiting, S., Rowsey, K., & Belisly, J. (2014). Assessing the relationship between intelligence and the PEAK relational training system. Research In Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8(9), 1208– 1213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.05.005.

 

Gore, N. J., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Murphy, G. (2010). The relationship between intellectual functioning and relational perspective-taking. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 10, 1–17.

 

Moran, L., Stewart, I., McElwee, J., & Ming, S. (2010). Brief report: The training and assessment of relational precursors and abilities (TARPA): A preliminary analysis. Journal of Autism and Devel- opmental Disorders, 40(9), 1149–1153. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0968-0.

 

O’Hora, D., Pelaez, M., & Banres-Holmes, D. (2005). Derived relational responding and performance on verbal subtests of the WAIS-III. The Psychological Record, 55, 155–175. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395504.

 

O’Hora, D., Pelaez, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Rae, G., Robinson, T., & Chaudhary, T. (2008). Temporal relations and intelligence: Correlating relational performance with performance on the WAIS-III. The Psychological Record, 58, 569–583. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395638.

 

O’Toole, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., Murphy, C., O’Connor, J., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2009). Relational flexibility and intelligence: Extending the remit of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 9, 1–17.

Evidence that Relational Skills Can Be Enhanced and Generalize to Novel Situations

Barnes-Holmes, Y. Barnes-Holmes, D. Roche, B, & Smeets, P. M. (2001). Exemplar training and a derived transformation of function in accordance with symmetry. The Psychological Record, 51, 287- 308.

 

Barnes-Holmes, Y. Barnes-Holmes, D. Roche, B, & Smeets, P. M. (2001). Exemplar training and a derived transformation of function in accordance with symmetry II. The Psychological Record, 51, 589-603.

 

Stewart, I, Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B. & Smeets, P. M. (2001). Generating derived relational networks via the abstraction of common physical properties: A possible model of analogical reasoning. The Psychological Record, 51, 381-408.

 

Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B. & Smeets, P. M. (2001). The development of self and perspective-taking: A Relational frame analysis. Behavior Development Bulletin, 1, 42-45.

 

Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (2004). Establishing relational responding in accordance with opposite as generalized operant behavior in young children. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4, 559–586.

 

Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & McHugh, L. (2004). Teaching derived relational responses to young children. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 1(1), 3-12.

 

Berens, N., & Hayes, S. (2007). Arbitrarily applicable comparative relations: Experimental evidence for a relational operant. Journal of Applied Behaviour Analysis, 40(1), 45–71. https://doi.org/10.1901/ jaba.2007.7-06.

 

Gomez, S., Lopez, F., Martin, C. B., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2007). Exemplar training and a derived transformation of functions in accordance with symmetry and equivalence. The Psychological Record, 57, 273–293. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395577.

 

Luciano, C., Becerra, I., & Valverde, M. (2007). The role of multiple-exemplar training and naming in establishing derived equivalence in an infant. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87(3), 349–365. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2007.08-06.

 

McKeel, A. N., Dixon, M. R., Daar, J. H., Rowsey, K. E., & Szekely, S. (2015). Evaluating the efficacy of the PEAK relational training system using a randomized controlled trial of children with autism. Journal of Behavioral Education, 24, 230–241. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-015-9219-y.

 

Rosales, R., Rehfeldt, R. A, Lovett, S. (2011). Effects of multiple exemplar training on the emergence of derived relations in preschool children learning a second language. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 61–74.

 

Ruiz, F. J., & Luciano, C. (2011). Cross- domain analogies as relating derived relations among two separate relational networks. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95(3), 369– 385. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2011.95-369

FAST Research Evidence

The following publications represent a sample of papers that provide a background to the rationale of the FAST (Function Acquisition Speed Test) method:

 

Cummins, J., & Roche, B. (2020). Quantifying differential within-class stimulus relatedness using the Function Acquisition Speed Test. Behavioral Processes.

 

Tyndall, I., Curtis, A., Cummins, J., & Roche, B. (2019). The Function Acquisition Speed Test (FAST) as a measure of verbal stimulus relations in the context of condom use. The Psychological Record 69, 107-115.

 

Cummins, J., Roche, B., Tyndall, I., & Cartwright, A. (2018). The relationship between differential stimulus relatedness and implicit test effect sizes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 118, 24-38.

 

Cartwright, A., Roche, B., Gogarty, M., O’Reilly, A. & Stewart, I. (2016). Using a Modified Function Acquisition Speed Test (FAST) for Assessing Implicit Gender Stereotypes. The Psychological Record, 66, 223-233.

 

O’Reilly, A., Roche, B., Ruiz, M., Ryan, A., & Campion, G. (2013). A Function Acquisition Speed Test for equivalence relations (FASTER). The Psychological Record, 63, 707-724.

 

O’Reilly, A., Roche, B., Ruiz, M. R., Tyndall, I. & Gavin, A. (2012). The Function Acquisition Speed Test (FAST): A behavior-analytic implicit test for assessing stimulus relations. The Psychological Record, 62, 507–528.

 

Gavin, A., Roche, B., Ruiz, M. R. & Hogan, M., & O’Reilly, A, (2012). A behavior-analytically modified Implicit Association Test for measuring the sexual categorization of children. The Psychological Record, 62, 55-68.

 

Roche, B., & O’Reilly, A., Gavin, A., Ruiz, M. R. & Arancibia, G. (2012). Using behavior-analytic implicit tests to assess sexual interests among normal and sex-offender populations. Socioaffective Neuroscience and Psychology, 2, 17335 – https://doi.org/ 10.3402/snp.v2i0.17335